© 2024 St. Louis Public Radio
Play Live Radio
Next Up:
0:00
0:00
0:00 0:00
Available On Air Stations

On Science: Environment affects I.Q.

This article first appeared in the St. Louis Beacon: Nowhere has the influence of environment on the expression of genetic traits led to more controversy than in studies of I.Q. scores. I.Q. is a controversial measure of general intelligence based on a written test that many feel to be biased toward white middle-class America. However well or poorly I.Q. scores measure intelligence, a person’s I.Q. score has been believed for some time to be determined largely by his or her genes.

How did science come to that conclusion?

Scientists measure the degree to which genes influence a multigene trait by using an off-putting statistical measure called the variance. Variance is defined as the square of the standard deviation (a measure of the degree-of-scatter of a group of numbers around their mean value), and has the very desirable property of being additive — that is, the total variance is equal to the sum of the variances of the factors influencing it.

What factors can contribute to the total variance of I.Q. scores? There are three:

  1. The first factor is variation at the gene level, some gene combinations leading to higher I.Q. scores than others.
  2. The second factor is variation at the environmental level, some environments leading to higher I.Q. scores than others.
  3. The third factor is what a statistician calls the covariance, the degree to which environment affects genes.

The degree to which genes influence a trait like I.Q., the heritability of I.Q., is given the symbol H and is defined simply as the fraction of the total variance that is genetic.

So how heritable is I.Q.?

Geneticists estimate the heritablity of I.Q. by measuring the environmental and genetic contributions to the total variance of I.Q. scores. The environmental contributions to variance in I.Q. can be measured by comparing the I.Q. scores of identical twins reared together with those reared apart (any differences should reflect environmental influences). The genetic contributions can be measured by comparing identical twins reared together (which are 100 percent genetically identical) with fraternal twins reared together (which are 50 percent genetically identical). Any differences should reflect genes, as twins share identical prenatal conditions in the womb and are raised in virtually identical environmental circumstances, so when traits are more commonly shared between identical twins than fraternal twins, the difference is likely genetic.

When these sorts of “twin studies” have been done in the past, researchers have uniformly reported that I.Q. is highly heritable, with values of H typically reported as being around 0.7 (a very high value). While it didn’t seem significant at the time, almost all the twins available for study over the years have come from middle-class or wealthy families.*

The study of I.Q. has proven controversial, because I.Q. scores are often different when social and racial groups are compared. What is one to make of the observation that I.Q. scores of poor children measure lower as a group than do scores of children of middle-class and wealthy families? This difference has led to the controversial suggestion by some that the poor are genetically inferior.

What should we make of such a harsh conclusion? To make a judgment, we need to focus, for a moment, on the fact that these measures of the heritability of I.Q. have all made a critical assumption, one to which population geneticists, who specialize in these sorts of things, object strongly. The assumption is that environment does not affect gene expression, so that covariance makes no contribution to the total variance in I.Q. Scores — that is, that the covariance contribution to H is zero.

Recent studies have allowed a direct assessment of this assumption. Importantly, it proves to be flat wrong.

In November 2003, researchers reported an analysis of twin data from a study carried out in the late 1960s. The National Collaborative Prenatal Project, funded by the National Institutes of Health, enrolled nearly 50,000 pregnant women, most of them black and quite poor, in several major U.S. cities. Researchers collected abundant data, and gave the children I.Q. tests seven years later. Although not designed to study twins, this study was so big that many twins were born, 623 births. Seven years later, 320 of these pairs were located and given I.Q. tests. This thus constitutes a huge “twin study,” the first ever conducted of I.Q. among the poor.

When the data were analyzed, the results were unlike any ever reported. The heritability of I.Q. was different in different environments! Most notably, the influence of genes on I.Q. was far less in conditions of poverty, where environmental limitations seem to block the expression of genetic potential. Specifically, for families of high socio-economic status, H=0.72, much as reported in previous studies, but for families raised in poverty, H=0.10, a very low value, indicating genes were making little contribution to observed I.Q. scores. The lower a child’s socio-economic status, the less impact genes had on I.Q.

These data say, with crystal clarity, that the genetic contributions to I.Q. don’t mean much in an impoverished environment.

How does poverty in early childhood affect the brain? Neuroscientists reported in 2008 that many children growing up in very poor families experience poor nutrition and unhealthy levels of stress hormones, both of which impair their neural development, effecting language development and memory for the rest of their lives.

Clearly, improvements in the growing and learning environments of poor children can be expected to have a major impact on their I.Q. scores. Additionally, these data argue that the controversial differences reported in mean I.Q scores between racial groups may well reflect no more than poverty, and are no more inevitable.

Copyright Txtwriter Inc.

'On science'

George B. Johnson is bringing his "On Science" column to the St Louis Beacon. This column looks at scientific issues and explains them in an accessible manner. There is no dumbing down in Johnson's writing, rather he uses analogy and precise terms to open the world of science to others.

Johnson, Ph.D., professor emeritus of Biology at Washington University, has taught biology and genetics to undergraduates for more than 30 years. Also professor of genetics at Washington University’s School of Medicine, Johnson is a student of population genetics and evolution, renowned for his pioneering studies of genetic variability.

He has authored more than 50 scientific publications and seven texts, including "BIOLOGY" (with botanist Peter Raven), "THE LIVING WORLD" and a widely used high school biology textbook, "HOLT BIOLOGY."

As the founding director of The Living World, the education center at the St Louis Zoo, from 1987 to 1990, he was responsible for developing innovative high-tech exhibits and new educational programs.